• Ei tuloksia

Metabolic state as a modulator of neural event-related potentials for food stimuli in an implicit association test

N/A
N/A
Info
Lataa
Protected

Academic year: 2022

Jaa "Metabolic state as a modulator of neural event-related potentials for food stimuli in an implicit association test"

Copied!
11
0
0

Kokoteksti

(1)

DSpace https://erepo.uef.fi

Rinnakkaistallenteet Terveystieteiden tiedekunta

2019

Metabolic state as a modulator of

neural event-related potentials for food stimuli in an implicit association test

Lahtinen, Aapeli

Elsevier BV

Tieteelliset aikakauslehtiartikkelit

© The Authors

CC BY http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2019.112589

https://erepo.uef.fi/handle/123456789/7945

Downloaded from University of Eastern Finland's eRepository

(2)

Contents lists available atScienceDirect

Physiology & Behavior

journal homepage:www.elsevier.com/locate/physbeh

Metabolic state as a modulator of neural event-related potentials for food stimuli in an implicit association test

Aapeli Lahtinen

a

, Kristiina Juvonen

b

, Anja Lapveteläinen

b

, Marjukka Kolehmainen

b

, Mikko Lindholm

c

, Heikki Tanila

d

, Teuvo Kantanen

e

, Sanna Sinikallio

f

, Leila Karhunen

b

, Johanna Närväinen

c,⁎

aVTT Technical Research Centre Of Finland, P.O. Box 1300, 33101 Tampere, Finland

bDepartment of Clinical Nutrition, Institute of Public Health and Clinical Nutrition, University of Eastern Finland, P.O. Box 1627, 70211 Kuopio, Finland

cVTT Technical Research Centre of Finland, P.O. Box 1199, 70211 Kuopio, Finland

dA. I. Virtanen Institute, University of Eastern Finland, P.O. Box 1627, 70211 Kuopio, Finland

eBusiness School, University of Eastern Finland, P.O. Box 1627, 70211 Kuopio, Finland

fSchool of Educational Sciences and Psychology, University of Eastern Finland, P.O. Box 111, 80101 Joensuu, Finland

A R T I C L E I N F O

Keywords:

Food ERP IAT Implicit Neural responses

A B S T R A C T

The Implicit Association Test (IAT) has become a ubiquitous measure of implicit associations or preferences in severalfields of research, including research related to food choices. The neural dynamics of the IAT have been explored in several contexts, but in a food-related IAT with stimuli of natural motivational value they are yet to be studied. Additionally, the effect of metabolic state on them is poorly known.

The present study examined the event-related potentials (ERP) in healthy non-obese females (n= 32) while they performed a food-related IAT in two sessions, in a fasted state and after a meal.

The results showed differences in the ERP components N400, P3 and LPP by congruence categories.

Additionally, the individual N400 and LPP deflections correlated strongly with individual IAT effects. ERP de- flections were weaker in the fasted state than after the meal despite greater implicit hedonic motivation towards food in the fasted state.

In conclusion, the results suggest that ERPs reflect the IAT effect. The N400, P3 and LPP components were evoked in a food-related IAT in a similar way observed in IAT tests in other contexts, reflecting a difference in meaning and motivation between congruence categories. The strong correlations of individual IAT effect with individual N400 and LPP deflections further suggests that the food-related IAT effect strength reflects the size of implicit food bias seen in neural deflections. Moreover, fasting increased implicit hedonic motivation towards food, but likely reduced cognitive resources at the same time. This could have made it harder to determine the value of novel, task-relevant stimuli, whereas it became easier postprandially and with practice.

1. Introduction

Food is a natural source of reward and as such a motivator ex- plaining and driving much of human action. The anticipated reward of food attracts our attention, which can be detected as a change in phy- siological activity such as neural deflections and can be modulated by homeostatic needs [1,2]. Furthermore, in recent years the dualistic nature of food reward, involving both implicit and explicit processing, has drawn interest [3]. Explicit processes of food reward are considered to be consciously observed, whereas implicit processes are not always experienced consciously [3]. Psychological and neurophysiological

methods to assess implicit and explicit processes are available, such as the Implicit Association Test (IAT) [4] and electroencephalography (EEG). These methods can broaden our understanding of our motiva- tional decision-making processes and the effect of metabolic state to it.

The IAT was designed to assess implicit preferences in a choice si- tuation between two target categories (e.g., food vs. non-food) and two associated evaluations or actions (in this case, approach vs. avoid) [4,5]. Response time difference between congruent (‘Food—Approach’

and ‘Non-food—Avoid’) and incongruent (‘Food—Avoid’ and ‘Non- food—Approach’) categorization situations produces the IAT effect, or D-score, which is considered to measure how strongly the concepts are

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2019.112589

Received 15 January 2019; Received in revised form 14 June 2019; Accepted 22 June 2019

Corresponding author at: VTT Technical Research Centre Of Finland, Microkatu 1, P.O. Box 1199, 70211 Kuopio, Finland.

E-mail address:johanna.narvainen@vtt.fi(J. Närväinen).

Physiology & Behavior 209 (2019) 112589

Available online 26 June 2019

0031-9384/ © 2019 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Inc. This is an open access article under the CC BY license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/BY/4.0/).

T

(3)

associated in memory [4,6]. When designing the IAT, researchers for- mulate a hypothesis that defines which categorization situations are predicted to be implicitly congruent and which as implicitly incon- gruent, and name these situations then accordingly. However, the result of the actual IAT test reveals the true direction of congruence. A posi- tive IAT effect is an indication of stronger association towards the congruent concept pair compared to the incongruent pair, as chosen by the researchers. The critical blocks of the IAT test used to calculate the IAT effect include dozens of trials and therefore a single categorization event has only a minor effect on the overall IAT score. The test has been used, for example, to evaluate self-esteem [7], stereotypes [8] and political behavior [9] as well as associations within various food con- texts [10–14].

Originally, IAT was designed to assess implicit evaluations (e.g. with

“positive”vs.“negative”attribute categories) [4,5]. Recently, the IAT has been modified to identify motivational tendencies towards objects by using“approach”and“avoid”attribute categories [15–17]. This IAT variant has been used for example in alcohol-related [15] as well as in food contexts [17,18]. Kraus and Piqueras-Fiszman [17] assessed im- plicit associations towards food products comparing motivational (“approach”vs.“avoid”) and evaluative (“positive”vs.“negative”) at- tribute categories in two separate IAT tests. The evaluative IAT did not reveal clear differences in preferences between sandwiches and sweets.

However, the result of the motivational IAT were in line with the results of explicit desire ratings between the food products in both hungry and reduced-hunger groups. The authors suggested that using“approach” and“avoid”attribute categories would be a sensitive method to assess implicit motivational responses towards food, whereas an evaluative IAT would be better suited to assess implicit liking.

It is still somewhat unclear whether the IAT captures implicit or explicit processes [19]. Therefore, attempts have been made to identify the mechanisms contributing to the IAT effect. Due to its high temporal resolution, electroencephalography (EEG) is one of the methods used to capture the time-course of neural processing during the IAT categor- ization events. These events can be seen as distinct stimuli or events that evoke neural responses, which are detected as event-related po- tentials (ERP) by time locking the events to the EEG. Comparison of ERPs during thefirst second following the stimulus presentation be- tween congruent and incongruent situations can reveal differences in neural processing related to the IAT effect.

The components of ERP are usually named according to their po- larity (N (negative) and P (positive)) and by either their order (e.g., N1, P2) or peak latency (e.g., N400 is a negative-going component centered around 400 ms), although some exceptions to this convention exist (e.g.

LPP or late parietal positivity) [20]. In a visual stimulus-processing context, certain late (≥300 ms) ERP components have been demon- strated to reflect neural processing related to stimulus salience or task relevance. The P3 component has been associated with stimulus eva- luation and categorization, attentional resource allocation and memory processes (for reviews, see [21,22]). The stimuli that initiate a P3 ac- tivity are varied, but they seem to be always motivationally significant.

The LPP (late parietal positivity) is a component related to the P3, re- flecting sustained attention towards motivationally salient stimuli [23].

Further, the N400 ERP components have been associated with conflict monitoring and processing of semantics and meaning (for reviews, see [24,25]).

To our knowledge there are no studies examining ERPs during a food-related IAT, although the neural dynamics of the implicit asso- ciations in an IAT have previously been studied in other contexts.

However, there is no consensus on which cognitive processes should be expected to be induced during an IAT task, regardless of context.

Exploratory IAT studies using event-related potentials (ERP) have found that N2, P3, N400 and LPP components differ by the target categories (e.g. self-positive/self-negative [26], natural/built environment [27], gay/straight [28]). On the other hand, some IAT studies have dis- covered the N2 and N400 components to be modulated by the

congruence categories of the IAT [26,28].

Approaching food and maintaining motivation-related associations towards food in memory is natural, as food is necessary for human survival. Whereas individual food items may not explicitly be as sought after as others, as a category food is more desirable than more neutral, everyday objects on an implicit level and attracts more attention [29].

Outside the IAT context, passive-viewing ERP studies have found dif- ferences in the P3 and LPP components between food and non-food stimuli [30–34], suggesting a greater motivational attention towards food stimuli. Therefore, it is of interest to find out whether similar neural activity can be found in a food-related IAT as has been found in these earlier food-related ERP-studies. Hunger modulates our attention and evaluative motivation to food. Food-related IAT studies (without ERPs) [35,36] using food words as stimuli have discovered greater re- action time difference between the test categories (food/non-food words as targets + pleasant/unpleasant attribute categories) in a fasted state compared to a postprandial state. Outside the IAT context, me- tabolic state alters also ERP components in passive-viewing food studies [32,34,37].

Given the lack of electrophysiological studies on the IAT effect in a food context, the aim of this study was to investigate whether differ- ences in the IAT congruence categories and the IAT effect associate with the amplitudes of ERP components describing stimulus evaluation, motivational attention and conflict monitoring. In addition, we ex- amined the effect of metabolic state (i.e. fasted vs. fed) on the neural representations of implicit preferences for food. Our hypothesis was that the later ERP components P3, N400 and LPP are modulated during the IAT by both event congruence and the metabolic state of the par- ticipants. The components were expected to be more positive in the congruent condition and larger (P3 and LPP more positive, N400 more negative) in the fasted condition.

2. Participants and methods 2.1. Participants

A total of 32 healthy females participated in the study (Table 1). The inclusion criteria of the study participants were age between 20 and 40 years and body mass index (BMI) between 19 and 29 kg/m2. Ex- clusion criteria were as follows: Food allergies or intolerances, re- strictive diet (e.g. vegetarian, gluten-free diet), frequent breakfast skipping, marked changes in diet during the past six months to lose weight, medication (except oral contraceptives), chronic disease (e.g.

diabetes, eating disorder, celiac or neurological disease), smoking and male sex. Participants were recruited via internet-based calls within the students and personnel of the University of Eastern Finland, Savonia University of Applied Sciences and Kuopio University Hospital. A compensation of 50 euros was provided to all participants.

The study was carried out in accordance with the guidelines laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki, and the Ethical Committee of Northern Savo Hospital District, Kuopio, Finland approved all proce- dures involving human participants. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Table 1

Characteristics of the female study participants (n= 32).

Characteristic Mean (SD) Min–Max

Age (years) 24.3 (5.5) 20.0–40.0

Weight (kg) 64.7 (7.3) 50.6–83.1

Height (cm) 168.2 (7.1) 153.7–184.6

Body mass index (kg/m2) 23.0 (3.0) 19.2–29.3

SD, standard deviation.

(4)

2.2. Study design

Before participating in the study, volunteers were interviewed to confirm their eligibility to the study. At the end of the screening in- terview, study procedure and measurements were explained to the volunteers at general level to familiarize them with the study protocol.

However, due to the nature of implicit responses, the detailed objec- tives of the study were not revealed to the volunteers, since prior in- formation could have likely affected the responses and hence the re- liability of the results [11,38].

Participants were instructed to maintain their habitual diet, exercise routines and sleep habits as constant as possible during the previous days before the study visit, refrain from heavy exercise 12 h before the study visit, and avoid alcohol consumption for 24 h before the study visit. At the beginning of the visit, participants' height and weight were measured, and the duration of fast and sleep and alcohol consumption during the previous day were checked by an interview.

Study visits were conducted at the Laboratory of Sensory Science of the Institute of Public Health and Clinical Nutrition at the University of Eastern Finland between 9:00 and 13:00 h. Each visit included two computer-based test sessions; the first one was performed after an overnight (10−12h) fast (fasted state) and the second one 30 min after a pizza lunch of participant's choice (fed state) (pizza options: Hawaii, Tuna, Mozzarella and Vegetable; Dr. Oetker, Suomi Oy, Helsinki, Finland). During the 30 min, between the end of the pizza meal and the beginning of the second test session participants could read, play games or browse the Internet. Participants rated their subjective sensations of appetite (i.e. hunger, desire to eat, satiety, and fullness) on a visual analogue scale (VAS; 0–10 cm, where 0 = not at all and 10 = ex- tremely) before and after the lunch, i.e. in the fasted and fed state.

2.3. The implicit association test, IAT

The IAT test was run by the Inquisit software (Inquisit 4.0.5.0, Millisecond Software, Seattle, WA, USA) and used to examine implicit associations between food and non-food items. The IAT test included two binary categorization tasks, one target (food vs. non-food item) and one attribute (approach vs. avoid) category pair, which were combined in an association-congruent (food – approach and non-food item – avoid) and association-incongruent (food–avoid and non-food item– approach) way. The standardized reaction time difference between the congruent and incongruent categorization events compiled as the IAT effect, or D-score, which is considered as an implication of a person's implicit bias. A positive IAT effect is interpreted as an indication of stronger association between the congruent concept pairings compared to the incongruent pairings. The IAT effect score has a possible range of

−2 to +2, which indicates the strength and the direction of the asso- ciation (−0.15 < D < 0.15 = little to no, D > 0.15 or D <−0.15 = slight, D > 0.35 or D <−0.35 = moderate, D > 0.65 or D <−0.65 = strong association). The raw IAT data were processed with a standard procedure in the Inquisit software to obtain thefinal IAT effect scores [6]. Due to the comparative nature of the IAT test, the resulting IAT effect scores cannot be interpreted as absolute preferences, but as relative ones between the target categories.

The test included 16 different target stimuli, 8 photographs for both target categories selected from a set of previously taken photographs shown inFig. 1[39]. The food and non-food item photos were matched in shape, colour and overall arrangement. All foods and non-food items were presented on a 15.6-in. screen, with a resolution of 1920 × 1080, on a grey background. Words (verbs in Finnish) describing the attribute categories were used as attribute stimuli, of which half indicated ap- proach-related (aspire, seek, favor, desire, choose, long for, need, take) and half avoid-related (refuse, avoid, restrict, reject, abandon, watch out, evade, withdraw) words. Both the target and attribute categories were presented in the top left and top right corners of the screen, and remained on the screen during the IAT test. Stimulus photos and words

were displayed successively in the center of the screen. Participants were instructed to categorize the stimulus photos and words as fast and accurately as possible by pressing one of two assigned response keys (left‘E' or right‘I') according to the category labels, while their in- dividual performance (reaction time and accuracy of the categorization (error rate)) was measured. The stimuli were displayed until the par- ticipant pressed a key and the interval between trials was 250 ms for all trials. Trials with a reaction time of > 10,000 ms were not included in further analysis. For incorrect responses they were given feedback by an

‘X' in the middle of the screen. A participant's error rate of > 10% was an exclusion criterion for the analysis of the IAT test results. One par- ticipant was excluded from the analysis.

The IAT test followed afixed block structure and included seven different blocks divided into practice (five blocks) and test trials (two blocks) (Fig. 2). After the separate practice blocks for target, attribute and their combination (20 trials in each), thefirst combined test block with 40 trials was presented. Then the categorization task changed between the blocks, continued with two practice blocks (20 trials) and ended with the second test block (40 trials). The order of the associa- tion-congruent and association-incongruent blocks was counter- balanced over participants. The duration of each block varied, as it was determined by the participant's reaction speed. Average duration of a single IAT was 2 min 50 s and with subjective sensation evaluations, the duration was 5 min on average.

2.4. Psychophysiological measurements and data analysis

The NeurOne monitoring system (Mega Electronics Ltd., Kuopio, Finland) was used to record scalp EEG. The EEG signal was digitized with a 500 Hz sampling rate from 64 Ag-AgCl electrodes mounted in an elastic cap (extended 10–20 system). Impedances were reduced to under 5 kΩfor all electrodes. After recording, signals werefiltered with a bandpass of 1–40 Hz and down-sampled to 200 Hz. Removal of arte- facts caused by blinks or eye movement was made using a regression algorithm. Electrodes T9 and T10 were used as a linked mastoid re- ference. Stimulus-locked epochs of −100 to 1000 ms relative to the stimulus presentation were extracted. All epochs containing signals exceeding 50 mV were rejected before averaging. For the events during the IAT test and subsequent picture evaluations, average ERP wave- forms were calculated for each electrode and grand average waveforms across all participants. The 100 ms pre-stimulus time window served as a baseline, and the amplitude of each ERP component was calculated as a difference to this baseline. Each participant's all waveforms of a cer- tain IAT trial (e.g., food–approach) were compared to each other and waveforms considerably different from the others (e.g., because the participant's thoughts were wandering) were discarded from further analyses. Altogether 5.8% of all ERP waveforms were discarded this way. ERP components have spatially different neural source locations and thus have different distributions across electrodes. Therefore, we divided electrodes into three clusters, parietal (Pz, P1, P2), central (C3, C4, Cz, C1, C2) and frontal (F3, F4, Fz, F1, F2). Based on previous IAT- ERP and food-ERP studies [26–28,31–34], we identified three ERP components from the grand average responses for further investigation:

P3 was extracted from 300 to 400 ms in the parietal cluster, N400 from 400 to 500 ms in the frontal, central and parietal clusters and LPP from 500 to 700 ms after the stimulus presentation in the parietal cluster.

Data analysis was carried out in Matlab (version 9.3; MathWorks, Natick MA, USA) using custom-made code.

2.5. Statistical analysis

The effects of metabolic state on subjective ratings and reaction times as well as the effect of congruence on reaction times were tested using paired-samplest-tests. Statistical analyses were performed sepa- rately for each ERP component of interest. To investigate the effect of metabolic state and the congruent and incongruent categories of the

A. Lahtinen, et al. Physiology & Behavior 209 (2019) 112589

3

(5)

IAT on measured variables, all mean amplitude ERP values were sub- mitted to a 2 (congruence: congruent, incongruent) × 2 (metabolic state: fasted, fed) ANOVA for repeated-measures. Interrelations be- tween ERP response differences (congruent–incongruent) and in- dividual IAT effect scores were analyzed by non-parametrical Spearman correlation analysis to take into account the possible distorting effect of outliers. However, parametrical Pearson correlation analysis was also concluded in comparison. Unless otherwise specified, the results are reported as means ± standard deviation (SD) with a valuep≤.05 (2-

tailed) as a criterion for the statistical significance.

The Matlab Statistics and Machine Learning Toolbox (version 11.2;

MathWorks, Natick MA, USA) was used for conducting statistical ana- lyses.

Fig. 1.The 16 photographs used as target stimuli in the IAT, 8 from each target category [39].

Fig. 2.Examples of the congruent (top row) and incongruent (bottom row) categorization task situations. The correct response category in each situation is indicated by a red box and the generated association between the targets and attributes is indicated by a pale blue box (the boxes are not displayed in the actual IAT session).

Stimulus photos were categorized by corresponding target categories (“Food”or“Non-food”) and stimulus words were categorized by corresponding attribute categories (e.g., word“aspire”to category“Approach”, word“abandon to category“Avoid”). Words were presented in Finnish.(For interpretation of the references to colour in thisfigure legend, the reader is referred to the web version of this article.)

(6)

3. Results

3.1. Explicit measures: appetite ratings

As expected, the participants' subjective appetite ratings changed significantly from the fasted to the fed state, which indicated a sig- nificant change in the metabolic state (Table 2). The levels of hunger and desire to eat were higher whereas satiety and fullness levels were lower in the fasted state compared to the fed state.

3.2. Behavioral measures: IAT

In the fasted state, average reaction times (RT) were shorter in the congruent than in the incongruent category (Table 3). This difference was present also in the fed state, even though the RTs in both categories were shorter compared to the fasted state. The RT differences reflected the group level average IAT effect scores, which were positive in both metabolic states, indicating that there was a group level preference for food over non-food items. This preference was moderate (D > 0.35) in the fasted state and slight (0.15 < D < 0.35) after the meal.

3.3. Neural responses: ERPs

In the ERP time series, the differences in the metabolic state and congruence categories were observed from ca. 200 ms after the stimulus onset (Fig. 3). The effect of metabolic state was present in all brain regions included into further analysis (frontal, central, parietal), whereas congruence had an effect especially in the parietal area. N400 responses were analyzed in all three cortical regions, but statistical analyses of P3 and LPP were performed only for the parietal area where the effects were hypothesized to occur.

The averaged ERPs across participants is illustrated inFig. 3, while Fig. 4shows participants' (n= 32) individual changes by congruence category and metabolic state. The direction of change was mostly consistent especially in the ERPs at the parietal area by congruence categories and in the parietal N400 and LPP by metabolic state. For the congruence effect, larger N400 amplitudes (i.e. more negative, as the N400 is a negative-going component) were detected in the incongruent than congruent category in 20/20/29 out of 32 participants (frontal/

central/parietal regions; Fig. 4 a, b and c). In contrast, parietal P3 amplitudes were larger in the congruent condition in 28 participants (Fig. 4d) and parietal LPP amplitudes in 27 participants (Fig. 4e). For

the effect of metabolic state, larger (more negative) N400 amplitudes were detected in 17/21/26 participants (frontal/central/parietal re- gions;Fig. 4f, g and h) in the fasted state compared to the fed state. On the other hand, parietal P3 (Fig. 4i) and LPP (Fig. 4j) amplitudes were larger (more positive) in the fed state in 22 and 28 participants, re- spectively.

In the statistical analyses for the analyzed ERPs, no significant in- teraction effect of congruence × metabolic state was found, indicating that the metabolic state effect did not have an influence on the con- gruence effect and vice versa. Therefore, in the following sections only main effects are described in more detail.

N400: The predicted congruence main effect was observed in the central (F1,31= 6.01,p= .020) and parietal (F1,31= 28.46,p< .001) clusters, with larger (relatively more negative) amplitudes in the in- congruent compared to the congruent category. However, no con- gruence effect was seen in the frontal cluster (p= .46). Furthermore, the predicted main effect of the metabolic state was observed in central (F1,31= 8.96, p= .005) and parietal (F1,31= 30.41, p< .001) clus- ters, with larger (relatively more negative) amplitudes in the fasted compared to the fed state. No metabolic state effect was found in the frontal cluster (p= .73).

Additionally, we examined the possible effect of consecutive com- ponents' amplitudes to each other [20]. To investigate whether the parietal N400 might have been affected by the previous P3 component, we ran an additional pairedt-test on the congruent-incongruent and fasted-fed differences on N400-P3. The comparative difference between N400 and P3 amplitudes were significant by metabolic state (p< .001), but not by congruence (p= .77).

P3: Analysis of the congruence effect in the parietal P3 component revealed a significant difference between the congruence categories (F1,31= 23.52, p < .001), with larger (more positive) amplitudes for the congruent category. A main effect for metabolic state was dis- covered as well (F1,31= 4.37,p= .045), with larger (more positive) amplitudes in the fed compared to the fasted state.

LPP: The parietal LPP ERP component also displayed a congruence main effect (F1,31) = 25.71, p < .001), with larger (more positive) amplitudes in the congruent category. Further, a main effect for the metabolic state was found (F1,31= 28.17, p < .001), such that am- plitudes were larger (more positive) in the fed state.

Similarly to the N400, the LPP might have been influenced by the temporally previous parietal N400 component. An additional pairedt- test was performed on the congruent-incongruent and fasted-fed dif- ferences on LPP-N400. The comparative difference between LPP and N400 amplitudes were found to be non-significant by metabolic state (p= .39) and by congruence (p= .12).

3.4. The effect of individual implicit association

The Spearman correlation analysis between the IAT effect values and ERP measures (Fig. 5) revealed the following interconnections:

N400: The central N400 amplitude difference (con- gruent–incongruent) was found to have a significant positive correla- tion with the IAT effect values in the fasted state. For the parietal N400 Table 2

Ratings of subjective appetite sensations (mean (SD)) in the fasted and fed states (n= 32 females).

Subjective sensation Fasted state Fed state p-valuea

Hunger 5.2 (2.1) 0.5 (0.7) p < .001

Desire to eat 6.0 (1.9) 1.6 (1.5) p < .001

Satiety 2.4 (2.3) 8.6 (0.8) p < .001

Fullness 2.0 (2.0) 7.9 (1.3) p < .001

a Paired samplest-test.

Table 3

Average reaction times (mean (SD), in milliseconds) and IAT effect values from the IAT test.

Statistics Metabolic state Overall mean Congruent Incongruent p-valuea

Reaction time Fasted 930 (460) 860 (450) 1000 (460) p < .001

Fed 770 (280) 740 (260) 810 (290) p < .001

p-valuea p < .001 p< .001 p < .001

IAT effect Fasted 0.47 (0.40)

Fed 0.33 (0.35)

p-valuea p= .03

SD, standard deviation.

a Paired samples t-test.

A. Lahtinen, et al. Physiology & Behavior 209 (2019) 112589

5

(7)

congruent–incongruent difference, there was a strong positive correla- tion with the IAT effect both in the fasted and in the fed state. No correlations were found for the frontal N400.

P3: No correlations were found between the P3 and the IAT effect in either the fasted or the fed state.

LPP: The LPP difference (congruent–incongruent) was found to have a significant positive correlation with the IAT effect in the fed state.

For comparison, we concluded a parametric Pearson correlation analysis, which in addition to the significant results of the Spearman's correlation revealed a significant positive correlation with the LPP difference and the IAT effect in the fasted state (p= .014).

4. Discussion

The present study examined how the ERP components are modu- lated by the congruence categories of the IAT and whether there is a relationship between the ERP measures and the IAT effect, i.e. the IAT effect. Furthermore, this study examined the effect of metabolic state, i.e. the influence of fasted and fed state on food-related implicit pre- ferences and ERP markers measured during the IAT. As hypothesized, the main ERP amplitudes were modulated by both event congruence and the metabolic state of the participants, being more positive in the congruent categories and in the fed state.

4.1. ERP components and IAT effect

The negative central-parietal N400 component was larger in the present study in the incongruent compared to the congruent category.

This result is in line with the processing of meaning and conflict sen- sitivity qualities related to the N400 component (for review, see [25]).

Similar results were also reported in previous IAT-ERP studies sur- veying neural bases of other associations such as self-positivity [26] and gay/straight bias [28]. The result was interpreted by Williams and Themanson as reflecting the incongruence in meaning between the target and attribute categories.

P3 amplitudes increased in the congruent IAT trials compared to the incongruent trials in the present study. The parietal P3 is known to reflect stimulus evaluation and categorization, and its amplitude in- creases by arousal level and the task-relevance or other motivational significance of the stimulus (for reviews, see [21,22]. Thisfinding is in line with the results of the available ERP studies that found a parietal P3 enhancement in visual food cue studies [30–32,34,40], and attitude –association IAT tests [26,27,41]. Food is a motivationally important stimulus and via the IAT also reflects in task-motivation that therefore evokes the P3 component.

The parietal LPP component was higher in congruent compared to incongruent categories in the present study. Somewhat similarly to the P3, the LPP reflects sustained attention towards motivationally salient stimuli and is involved in memory formation [23]. Higher amplitudes Fig. 3.The effect of congruence and metabolic state in grand mean averages of ERPs in the selected electrode clusters representing frontal, central and parietal regions. The time-windows for the components taken into statistical analyses are highlighted in colors (P3 = green, N400 = blue, LPP = cyan yellow). (For inter- pretation of the references to colour in thisfigure legend, the reader is referred to the web version of this article.)

(8)

for arousing stimuli have been construed as indicating enhanced memory processing performance for these stimuli. Several IAT-ERP studies have found modulation by congruence in the LPP (or an LPP- like deflection) with target stimuli related to race [42], sexual or- ientation [28] and gender [43].

We found several significant correlations between individual IAT effect and congruence category related ERP amplitudes. The N400 and LPP amplitude differences (congruent - incongruent) were stronger in individuals with higher IAT effect. This suggests that individuals showing pronounced implicit preference towards food showed higher LPP amplitudes in congruent compared to incongruent situations and larger N400 amplitudes in incongruent compared to congruent situa- tions. In other words, stronger food bias evokes stronger conflict monitoring towards avoiding food objects as well as greater effort in constructing a meaning for combining such options, both seen in the N400. The strength of food bias also generates increased motivation towards food stimuli, seen in the LPP. This suggests that food bias strength observed in the IAT effect dominates ERP differences.

These results are also in line with the results reported in previous IAT research in other contexts. Schindler et al. [44] observed in a doping attitude IAT study a similar correlation as in the present study between the IAT effect and the parietal LPP. On the other hand, Wil- liams and Themanson [28] and Wu et al. [26] compared reaction time differences (incongruent-congruent) to ERP amplitude differences (congruent-incongruent) and reported reverse U-shaped correlations for

reaction time differences and ERP components (N400 and LPP; and P3, respectively). However, it remains unsolved whether the direction of correlation differs due to the nature of the IAT or variations in the statistical methods.

In summary, these results indicate that on the group level, the N400, P3 and LPP components are evoked in a food-related IAT task in a si- milar way to IAT tasks reported in other contexts. Additionally, on the individual level, the N400 and the LPP correlated strongly with the IAT effect, suggesting that the IAT effect reflects neural deflections of im- plicit food bias.

4.2. Effect of metabolic state

Participants described themselves to be significantly hungrier be- fore the meal than postprandially (Table 2), which indicated that a change in the metabolic state had occurred. As motivation towards food was predicted to be higher in the fasted state, fasting was hypothesized to modulate ERP components sensitive to motivationally salient stimuli (P3, LPP) and associated with processing of meaning and conflict monitoring qualities (N400).

Surprisingly, the motivation-related ERP components (P3, LPP) were significantly stronger in the fed compared to the fasted state. On the other hand, compared to the fed state, response-based measures, i.e.

IAT reaction times and the IAT effect, showed a stronger implicit pre- ference of the participants towards food when fasting (Table 3), which Fig. 4.Changes in the studied ERP components in individual participants by congruence category (top row) and metabolic state (bottom row). The blue lines describe decrease and the red, dashed lines increase in ERP amplitudes. In the negative-going N400 component, a smaller amplitude describes a larger component, whereas in the positive-going P3 and LPP components a larger component is described by a larger amplitude. The blue bars on the sides represent the grand mean amplitudes with standard deviations. CON = congruent, INC = incongruent, FAS = fasted. (For interpretation of the references to colour in thisfigure legend, the reader is referred to the web version of this article.)

A. Lahtinen, et al. Physiology & Behavior 209 (2019) 112589

7

(9)

is in line with previous literature [35,36]. This change in the implicit preference seemed to be opposite to those seen in the P3 and LPP components, initially suggesting that metabolic state does not have a parallel effect on presumed behavioral and ERP indices of implicit preferences towards food. However, in non-IAT studies observing the effect of fasting to food-related ERPs, viewing food stimuli have elicited stronger ERPs in a fasted compared to a fed state contrary to the ERP results seen in the current study. Increases have been detected in par- ietal P3 [30–32,34,37] and LPP components [31,37]. These results would seem to disagree with the ERP results seen in this study and suggest that the motivation towards food stimuli is increased in a de- prived state.

The N400 component was larger in the fasted state. This likely re- flects heightened conflict monitoring or increased meaning processing [25]. To our knowledge, the effect of metabolic state to the N400 component has not been previously studied, making this a novel finding.

There are several potential explanations for the differences in the results between the current study and the studies described above. First, implicit preferences are comprised of hedonic and cognitive neural systems, which influence choices uniquely and are modified differently by availability of cognitive resources [45–47]. Second, the IAT is cog- nitively more demanding than passive viewing paradigms [48], mod- ifying motivation-related ERPs differently. Third, fasting reduces cog- nitive resources and affects the perceived difficulty of the cognitive task at hand, which in turn have an effect on the implicit hedonic and cognitive bases of choice seen in task performance (for review, see [49]), IAT effect [35,36] and neural activation [50–52]. On the other hand, in the present study participants completed the same IAT test both before and after a lunch, which may result in a better performance and altered neural deflections during the second IAT task simply due to a learning effect and therefore participants likely performed the task more efficiently [53].

In the present study, the implicit preference to food, as measured by

the IAT effect, was stronger in the fasted state than postprandially (Table 3) and therefore produced results counterintuitive to those seen in neural activity, which implied a higher arousal in the fed state (Fig. 3). Trendel and Werle [45] found that overall implicit attitudes to food are driven not only by a hedonic basis of implicit attitudes, but also by an automatic cognitive basis distinct and independent from the hedonic component, which has also been found in other contexts [47,54]. Thisfinding is in accordance with dual process models em- ployed in thefield of social psychology (see [55]), which propose an existence of automatic and controlled processes that contribute in be- havior. Trendel and Werle also found the two systems to react differ- ently under high and low cognitive load, with the food's palatability driving food choices under high load and the cognitive implicit basis having a greater impact when greater cognitive capacity was available.

This result was in agreement with the‘attentional myopia’model of Mann and Ward [46] stating that restricted attention can lead to most salient cues stealing focus. Considering that in this study the fasting state influenced the implicit preferences, as shown in the IAT effect, differently to those seen in the motivation-related ERPs (P3, LPP), it might be that the implicit hedonic and cognitive processing operated differently under the load of fasting.

In the present study, the effect of fasting to ERP amplitudes was opposite to those observed in previous food-related ERP studies [32,34,37]. However, in contrast to this study, these studies im- plemented mainly passive viewing paradigms, which might have re- quired less concentration and cognitive resources than an active, re- sponse-based task such as the IAT. In fact, Coates and Campbell [48]

noticed that the IAT created such an extensive processing load that even obtrusive distractors during the test did not have an effect on attending the task. Further, the stimuli of the‘food’and‘non-food item’categories used in the IAT test of the current study were by design extremely si- milar. This increased difficulty of discrimination has been connected with increased attentional capture as well as decreased task perfor- mance compared to tasks with easier stimulus discrimination Fig. 5.Non-parametrical Spearman correlations between IAT effects (x-axis) and ERP amplitudes (y-axis) in fasted (top row) and fed (bottom row) states. Significant correlations (p< .05) were found for the central-parietal N400 in the fasted state, and for the parietal N400 and LPP components in the fed state.

(10)

[50,51,56]. Additionally, the parietal P3 decreases and anterior, con- flict monitoring N2 and stimulus evaluating P3a deflections increase with higher discrimination difficulty. Although frontal P3a amplitudes were not statistically analyzed in the present study, visual inspection suggests an effect of increased frontal and diminished parietal P3 de- flections in the fasted compared to the fed state (Fig. 3). This further suggests that the IAT test used in the current study was cognitively more difficult than the passive-viewing paradigms in previous food- related ERP studies, which in part explains an opposite effect of me- tabolic state on the parietal motivation-related P3 and LPP components.

Previous studies have shown that metabolic state modulates neural deflections observed during a cognitive task. Even though results have varied, fasting and a low blood sugar level have nevertheless demon- strated an impact on both cognitive task performance and the P3 ERP component related to memory indexing, attention, motivation and task- relevance (for reviews, see [49,57]). Although the literature on the subject is varied, there are indications of either task performance, neural activity or both during a cognitive task being reduced in a fasted state compared to a fed state, illustrating that cognitive tasks may be harder to perform when hungry. Longer reaction times observed during the IAT in the fasted state in our study also support this view.

As the fasted state reduces cognitive resources, these observations are in line with the work mentioned above relating to task dis- crimination difficulty [50,51,56]. As the parietal P3 weakens with in- creasing task difficulty, and fasting might make cognitive tasks even harder to execute as well as influence the P3 component, these two phenomena might exert a cumulative effect on the P3 (and possibly the LPP, which is known to share many qualities of the P3). Therefore, in a task including difficult discrimination between stimuli it might be hard to determine their task-relevance or motivational value.

In addition, learning may have further affected the task difficulty effect between fasted and fed states in this study. All participants completed the IAT testfirst in a fasted state and then postprandially.

This means that a learning effect was present despite the use of practice blocks and the postprandial task might have been easier to perform due to the participants being more familiar with the task. Therefore, this change in subjective competence may have added to the change seen in ERP amplitudes and contributed to the faster reaction times in the postprandial IAT [53].

The study group considered the possibility of a learning or famil- iarity effect when designing the study, but also considered the benefits of running both IAT sessions during a single study visit of each parti- cipant. Compared to measurements performed during two separate days, with a fasting period in between, using the current design the variability in the state of the participants (e.g. psychological and phy- siological) was minimized. This also enabled the use of the same EEG setup in both sessions, which eliminates cross-measurement variability, due to e.g. EEG impedance optimization and cap positioning, in the biosignals. All this amounted to better replicability of the measure- ments. As a drawback of this design, itfixed the fasted state with the first IAT session, making it difficult to separate learning effects from the effects of fasting. Practice blocks were used to minimize the learning effect, but the possibility of this effect cannot be ruled out completely.

Taking together, in this study, due to the difficulty of discrimination and high level of attentional capture of the IAT used in this study, motivation-related ERPs (P3, LPP) might have been influenced by he- donic motivation and simultaneously also strongly modulated by the task-related motivation towards the stimuli. Therefore, while fasting increased the hedonic value of the food stimuli as indicated in the IAT effect, this effect was not reflected in ERPs in which such effects were subdued by the effects of the relatively demanding task. The ERP am- plitudes were predominantly affected by the challenges raised by this IAT task with difficult stimulus discrimination, and the difficulty was even stronger in the fasted state, causing the processing of the cate- gorization events to be harder compared to the fed state.

Finally, there is a possibility that the ERPfindings are influenced by

each other. Additional statistical analyses indicated that N400 differ- ences by metabolic state were independent of the P3. However, as other relative changes in temporally adjacent ERP amplitudes were not sig- nificant, it is possible that a preceding component (for example, the N400 preceding the LPP) contributed to the changes observed in the following component (for example, changes in the LPP). It is also pos- sible, that an underlying larger wave could have influenced the com- ponent differences seen in this study. Future research could attempt to clarify this possible connection.

An intriguing question for future research is whether the hedonic and cognitive bases for implicit attitudes share a neural background seen in the P3 and LPP in this study. Another explanation would be that they originate from different neural locations, which merely manifest in the same timeframe and in the parietal area.

5. Conclusions

In summary, event-related potentials were observed to reflect the IAT effect in a food-related context. The N400, P3 and LPP components are evoked in a food-related IAT test in a similar way to IATs used in other contexts. Additionally, the individual N400 and LPP deflections correlated strongly with the individual IAT effect, further suggesting that the food-related IAT effect reflects implicit food bias detected in neural deflections. Furthermore, fasting increases implicit hedonic motivation towards food, but also simultaneously likely reduces cog- nitive resources. This likely makes the determination of the value of novel, task-relevant stimuli harder, whereas postprandially and with practice this determination becomes easier.

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Eeva Lajunen for her assistance in the study.

This work was funded by the Academy of Finland (286028, 290183), Tekes– the Finnish Funding Agency for Technology and Innovation (40322/13, 2834/31/2013) and Juho Vainio Foundation, Finland (201610293, 201710310).

Declarations of interest None.

References

[1] J.M. Born, S.G.T. Lemmens, F. Rutters, A.G. Nieuwenhuizen, E. Formisano, R. Goebel, M.S. Westerterp-Plantenga, Acute stress and food-related reward acti- vation in the brain during food choice during eating in the absence of hunger, Int. J.

Obes. 34 (2010) 172–181,https://doi.org/10.1038/ijo.2009.221.

[2] P.A. Smeets, A. Erkner, C. De Graaf, Cephalic phase responses and appetite, Nutr.

Rev. 68 (2010) 643–655,https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1753-4887.2010.00334.x.

[3] M.L. Kringelbach, A. Stein, T.J. van Hartevelt, The functional human neuroanatomy of food pleasure cycles, Physiol. Behav. 106 (2012) 307–316,https://doi.org/10.

1016/j.physbeh.2012.03.023.

[4] A.G. Greenwald, D.E. McGhee, J.L.K. Schwartz, Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the implicit association test, J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 74 (1998) 1464–1480,https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.74.6.1464.

[5] K.A. Lane, M.R. Banaji, B.A. Nosek, A.G. Greenwald, Understanding and Using What we Know (So Far) about the Method, Implicit Meas. Attitudes Proced. Controv, (2007), pp. 59–102,https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015575.

[6] A.G. Greenwald, B.A. Nosek, M.R. Banaji, Understanding and using the implicit association test: an improved scoring algorithm, J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 85 (2003) 197–216,https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.197.

[7] A.G. Greenwald, S.D. Farnham, Using the implicit association test to measure self- esteem and self-concept, J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 79 (2000) 1022–1038,https://doi.

org/10.1037/0022-3514.79.6.1022.

[8] J. Agerström, D.-O. Rooth, The role of automatic obesity stereotypes in real hiring discrimination, J. Appl. Psychol. 96 (2011) 790–805,https://doi.org/10.1037/

a0021594.

[9] S. Galdi, L. Arcuri, B. Gawronski, Automatic mental associations predict future choices of undecided decision-makers, Science (80-.). 321 (2008) 1100–1102, https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1160769.

[10] K. Ayres, M.T. Conner, A. Prestwich, P. Smith, Do implicit measures of attitudes incrementally predict snacking behaviour over explicit affect-related measures?

A. Lahtinen, et al. Physiology & Behavior 209 (2019) 112589

9

(11)

Appetite. 58 (2012) 835–841,https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2012.01.019.

[11] P. Bongers, A. Jansen, K. Houben, A. Roefs, Happy eating: the single target implicit association test predicts overeating after positive emotions, Eat. Behav. 14 (2013) 348–355,https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2013.06.007.

[12] A. Haynes, E. Kemps, R. Moffitt, Inhibitory self-control moderates the effect of changed implicit food evaluations on snack food consumption, Appetite. 90 (2015) 114–122,https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2015.02.039.

[13] K. Houben, A. Roefs, A. Jansen, Guilty pleasures. Implicit preferences for high calorie food in restrained eating, Appetite. 55 (2010) 18–24,https://doi.org/10.

1016/j.appet.2010.03.003.

[14] A. Roefs, A. Jansen, Implicit and explicit attitudes toward high-fat foods in obesity, J. Abnorm. Psychol. 111 (2002) 517–521,https://doi.org/10.1037//0021-843x.

111.3.517.

[15] T.P. Palfai, B.D. Ostafin, Alcohol-related motivational tendencies in hazardous drinkers: assessing implicit response tendencies using the modified-IAT, Behav. Res.

Ther. 41 (2003) 1149–1162,https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(03)00018-4.

[16] K.P. Lindgren, C. Neighbors, B.A. Teachman, M.L. Gasser, D. Kaysen, J. Norris, R.W. Wiers, Habit doesn't make the predictions stronger: implicit alcohol associa- tions and habitualness predict drinking uniquely, Addict. Behav. 45 (2015) 139–145,https://doi.org/10.1016/J.ADDBEH.2015.01.003.

[17] A.A. Kraus, B. Piqueras-Fiszman, Sandwich or sweets? An assessment of two novel implicit association tasks to capture dynamic motivational tendencies and stable evaluations towards foods, Food Qual. Prefer. 49 (2016) 11–19,https://doi.org/10.

1016/j.foodqual.2015.11.005.

[18] E. Kemps, M. Tiggemann, R. Martin, M. Elliott, Implicit approach-avoidance asso- ciations for craved food cues, J. Exp. Psychol. Appl. 19 (2013) 30–38,https://doi.

org/10.1037/a0031626.

[19] J. De Houwer, S. Teige-Mocigemba, A. Spruyt, A. Moors, Implicit measures: a normative analysis and review, Psychol. Bull. 135 (2009) 347–368,https://doi.org/

10.1037/a0014211.

[20] S.J. Luck, An Introduction to the Event-Related Potential Technique, a Bradford Book, (2005),https://doi.org/10.1118/1.4736938.

[21] A. Kok, On the utility of P3 amplitude as a measure of processing capacity, Psychophysiology. 38 (2001) 557–577,https://doi.org/10.1017/

s0048577201990559.

[22] J. Polich, Updating P300: an integrative theory of P3a and P3b, Clin. Neurophysiol.

118 (2007) 2128–2148,https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2007.04.019.

[23] J.K. Olofsson, S. Nordin, H. Sequeira, J. Polich, Affective picture processing: an integrative review of ERPfindings, Biol. Psychol. 77 (2008) 247–265,https://doi.

org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.11.006.

[24] J.R. Folstein, C. Van Petten, Influence of cognitive control and mismatch on the N2 component of the ERP: A review, Psychophysiology 0 (2007),https://doi.org/10.

1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00602.x.

[25] M. Kutas, K.D. Federmeier, Thirty years and counting:finding meaning in the N400 component of the event-related brain potential (ERP), Annu. Rev. Psychol. 62 (2011) 621–647,https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.131123.

[26] L. Wu, R. Gu, H. Cai, J. Zhang, Electrophysiological evidence for executive control and efficient categorization involved in implicit self-evaluation, Soc. Neurosci. 11 (2016) 153–163,https://doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2015.1044673.

[27] G.F. Healy, L. Boran, A.F. Smeaton, Neural patterns of the implicit association test, Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9 (2015) 1–16,https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00605.

[28] J.K. Williams, J.R. Themanson, Neural correlates of the implicit association test:

evidence for semantic and emotional processing, Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 6 (2011) 468–476,https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsq065.

[29] L.N. van der Laan, D.T.D. de Ridder, M.A. Viergever, P.A.M. Smeets, Thefirst taste is always with the eyes: a meta-analysis on the neural correlates of processing visual food cues, Neuroimage. 55 (2011) 296–303,https://doi.org/10.1016/j.

neuroimage.2010.11.055.

[30] I.M. Nijs, I.H. Franken, P. Muris, Food-related Stroop interference in obese and normal-weight individuals: behavioral and electrophysiological indices, Eat. Behav.

11 (2010) 258–265https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2010.07.002.

[31] I.M. Nijs, I.H. Franken, P. Muris, Food cue-elicited brain potentials in obese and healthy-weight individuals, Eat. Behav. 9 (2008) 462–470https://doi.org/10.

1016/j.eatbeh.2008.07.009.

[32] I.M. Nijs, P. Muris, A.S. Euser, I.H. Franken, Differences in attention to food and food intake between overweight/obese and normal-weight females under condi- tions of hunger and satiety, Appetite. 54 (2010) 243–254https://doi.org/10.1016/

j.appet.2009.11.004.

[33] D. Asmaro, F. Jaspers-Fayer, V. Sramko, I. Taake, P. Carolan, M. Liotti, Spatiotemporal dynamics of the hedonic processing of chocolate images in in- dividuals with and without trait chocolate craving, Appetite. 58 (2012) 790–799, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2012.01.030.

[34] C. Nikendei, H.C. Friederich, M. Weisbrod, S. Walther, A. Sharma, W. Herzog, S. Zipfel, S. Bender, Event-related potentials during recognition of semantic and pictorial food stimuli in patients with anorexia nervosa and healthy controls with varying internal states of hunger, Psychosom. Med. 74 (2012) 136–145https://doi.

org/10.1097/PSY.0b013e318242496a.

[35] L.D. Stafford, G. Scheffler, Hunger inhibits negative associations to food but not auditory biases in attention, Appetite. 51 (2008) 731–734,https://doi.org/10.

1016/j.appet.2008.04.020.

[36] B. Seibt, M. Häfner, R. Deutsch, Prepared to eat: how immediate affective and motivational responses to food cues are influenced by food deprivation, Eur. J. Soc.

Psychol. 37 (2007) 359–379,https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.365.

[37] J. Stockburger, R. Schmalzle, T. Flaisch, F. Bublatzky, H.T. Schupp, The impact of hunger on food cue processing: an event-related brain potential study, Neuroimage.

47 (2009) 1819–1829https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.04.071.

[38] E. Harmon-Jones, D.M. Amodio, L.R. Zinner, Social psychological methods of emotion elicitation, in: J.J.B. Coan, J.A. Allen (Eds.), Ser. Affect. Sci. Handb. Emot.

Elicitation Assess, Oxford University Press, New York, NY, US, 2007, pp. 91–105 http://psycnet.apa.org/record/2007-08864-006, Accessed date: 30 May 2018.

[39] S. Kaurijoki, J.T. Kuikka, E. Niskanen, S. Carlson, K.H. Pietilainen, U. Pesonen, J.M. Kaprio, A. Rissanen, J. Tiihonen, L. Karhunen, Association of serotonin transporter promoter regulatory region polymorphism and cerebral activity to vi- sual presentation of food, Clin. Physiol. Funct. Imaging 28 (2008) 270–276,https://

doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-097X.2008.00804.x\rCPF804(pii).

[40] J. Hofmann, E. Ardelt-Gattinger, K. Paulmichl, D. Weghuber, J. Blechert, Dietary restraint and impulsivity modulate neural responses to food in adolescents with obesity and healthy adolescents, Obesity. 23 (2015) 2183–2189,https://doi.org/

10.1002/oby.21254.

[41] M. Fleischhauer, A. Strobel, K. Diers, S. Enge, Electrophysiological evidence for early perceptual facilitation and efficient categorization of self-related stimuli during an implicit association test measuring neuroticism, Psychophysiology. 51 (2014) 142–151,https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12162.

[42] E. Hurtado, A. Haye, R. González, F. Manes, A. Ibáñez, Contextual blending of in- group/outgroup face stimuli and word valence: LPP modulation and convergence of measures, BMC Neurosci. 10 (2009) 69,https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2202- 10-69.

[43] C.E. Forbes, K.A. Cameron, J. Grafman, A. Barbey, J. Solomon, W. Ritter, D.S. Ruchkin, Identifying temporal and causal contributions of neural processes underlying the implicit association test (IAT), Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6 (2012) 1–18, https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00320.

[44] S. Schindler, W. Wolff, Cerebral correlates of automatic associations towards per- formance enhancing substances, Front. Psychol. 6 (2015) 1–9,https://doi.org/10.

3389/fpsyg.2015.01923.

[45] O. Trendel, C.O.C. Werle, Distinguishing the affective and cognitive bases of im- plicit attitudes to improve prediction of food choices, Appetite. 104 (2015) 33–43, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2015.10.005.

[46] T. Mann, A. Ward, Attention, self-control, and health Behaviors, Curr. Dir. Psychol.

Sci. 16 (2007) 280–283,https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00520.x.

[47] D.M. Amodio, P.G. Devine, Stereotyping and evaluation in implicit race bias: evi- dence for independent constructs and unique effects on behavior, J. Pers. Soc.

Psychol. 91 (2006) 652–661,https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.91.4.652.

[48] M.A. Coates, K.B. Campbell, Event-related potential measures of processing during an implicit association test, Neuroreport. 21 (2010) 1029–1033,https://doi.org/10.

1097/wnr.0b013e32833f5e7d.

[49] E.M. Benau, N.C. Orloff, E.A. Janke, L. Serpell, C.A. Timko, A systematic review of the effects of experimental fasting on cognition, Appetite. 77 (2014) 52–61,https://

doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2014.02.014.

[50] G.F. Hagen, J.R. Gatherwright, B.A. Lopez, J. Polich, P3a from visual stimuli: task difficulty effects, Int. J. Psychophysiol. 59 (2006) 8–14,https://doi.org/10.1016/j.

ijpsycho.2005.08.003.

[51] N. Benikos, S.J. Johnstone, S.J. Roodenrys, Varying task difficulty in the go/Nogo task: the effects of inhibitory control, arousal, and perceived effort on ERP com- ponents, Int. J. Psychophysiol. 87 (2013) 262–272,https://doi.org/10.1016/j.

ijpsycho.2012.08.005.

[52] A.J. Matthews, F.H. Martin, M. Garry, J.J. Summers, The behavioural and elec- trophysiological effects of visual task difficulty and bimanual coordination mode during dual-task performance, Exp. Brain Res. 198 (2009) 477–487,https://doi.

org/10.1007/s00221-009-1943-x.

[53] N. Benikos, S.J. Johnstone, S.J. Roodenrys, Short-term training in the go/Nogo task:

behavioural and neural changes depend on task demands, Int. J. Psychophysiol. 87 (2013) 301–312,https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2012.12.001.

[54] W.A. Cunningham, P.D. Zelazo, Attitudes and evaluations: a social cognitive neu- roscience perspective, Trends Cogn. Sci. 11 (2007) 97–104,https://doi.org/10.

1016/j.tics.2006.12.005.

[55] S. Chaiken, Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual-Process Theories in Social Psychology, Guilford Press, New York, NY, US, 1999.

[56] R. Sawaki, J.N.I. Katayama, Difficulty of discrimination modulates attentional capture by regulating attentional focus, J. Cogn. Neurosci. 21 (2009) 359–371, https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2008.21022.

[57] E.A. de Bruin, M.B. Gilsenan, Effects of food energy on cognitive performance: no support from event-related potentials (yet?), Br. J. Nutr. 101 (2008) 1047,https://

doi.org/10.1017/s0007114508051702.

Viittaukset

LIITTYVÄT TIEDOSTOT

Vuonna 1996 oli ONTIKAan kirjautunut Jyväskylässä sekä Jyväskylän maalaiskunnassa yhteensä 40 rakennuspaloa, joihin oli osallistunut 151 palo- ja pelastustoimen operatii-

The objective of conducting an extensive literature search on BFIs is to list the existing candidate BFIs for a specific food or food group and to provide available evi- dence for

Ympäristökysymysten käsittely hyvinvointivaltion yhteydessä on melko uusi ajatus, sillä sosiaalipolitiikan alaksi on perinteisesti ymmärretty ihmisten ja yhteiskunnan suhde, eikä

This issue of Agricultural and Food Science contains a selection of articles that focus on economics and policies related to agriculture, food and the environment.. The

The objectives of the study were: (a) to present the food consumption patterns in Finland over the period 1950-1991, (b) to estimate a demand system for a 18-category breakdown of

At this point in time, when WHO was not ready to declare the current situation a Public Health Emergency of In- ternational Concern,12 the European Centre for Disease Prevention

In addition, country can have an impact on food-related behaviour, not only because of differ- ences in food culture, but also because of differences in the extent to which

Extraversion has been related to positive affect both in a behavioral tendency to experience positive emotions as well as in neural correlates of processing positive emotional