• Ei tuloksia

Music as a hobby has been shown to have beneficial effects on general cogni-tive development. The mere exposure to sound has been shown to have sev-eral benefits on the brain in animal and human studies. For example, the amount of dopamine, important for the reward system, regulating sleep, mood, attention and learning, (Sutoo & Akiyama 2004), and the activity patterns of the glutamate receptors (Nichols et al. 2007) are enhanced by simple sound exposure. In the auditory cortical areas, sound exposure alters the regularity of the structure of the auditory cortex and enhances learning and memory (Angelucci et al. 2007). In several key brain areas like the hip-pocampus, hypothalamus and cortical areas, sound exposure increases the production of new neurons and nerve growth (Angelucci et al. 2007), making these areas more capable of adapting to new types of challenging tasks.

When children with music as their hobby are compared to their peers, several very general enhancements can be observed. For example,

correla-tions between music or sound perception skills have been found with spatial reasoning, reading, phonemic awareness and general intelligence in children (Nelson & Barresi 1989; Douglas & Willats 1994). In addition, correlations between musical training have been observed with verbal memory and flu-ency, visuospatial perception, attention allocation and different reading tasks in children. Further, the amount of musical training that a child has received correlates with general intelligence (Schellenberg 2006). Such findings speak for a strong correlation between general cognitive capabilities and musical education. These studies fail to show, however, the direction of causality, namely, whether the children with strong cognitive capabilities become inter-ested in music and want to pursue it as their hobby, continue it because of the success received due to their high cognitive capabilities, or whether musical training really enhances the children’s cognitive capabilities in a very general way.

The only way to answer this chicken and egg question is to perform ex-pensive and time-consuming interventional studies. In these studies, children are randomly allocated to groups that receive intensive high-level profes-sional training either music or another extracurricular activity, and their skill profiles are assessed prior to and after the training. There are several such interventional programs currently running in many countries. The first results clearly show that in two similar groups with similar before-training skill profiles, the group with musical training will achieve higher mathematical and spatial skills, reasoning skills, reading and writing skills, and skills of detecting changes in prosodic features of speech (Gardiner et al. 1996;

Rauscher et al. 1993; Gromko et al. 2005; Bilhartz et al. 2000; Costa-Giomi 2004; Thompson et al. 2004; Moreno et al. 2006, 2008). Thus it seems evi-dent that the chicken exists before the egg—children really do benefit from musical training irrespective of their prior cognitive capabilities, inherited musical skills, or family-related matters like social status or parents’ interest towards music.

Tallal and Gaab (2006) proposed that, in addition to specific effects like enhancement of the auditory system areas, two major general effects would be responsible for the higher cognitive skills after musical training. First, if children learn to control their attention, this ability leads to very general ef-fects in both learning and testing situations. Thus, learning to control volun-tary attention, learning to suppress involunvolun-tary shifts of attention, and learn-ing to shift one’s attention back to the original task quickly and effortlessly after an involuntary shift of attention are all key skills in working in noisy environments. This is an effect that music has in common with other extra-curricular activities, at least in some of them like theater, circus, and dance, activities where the attentive skills are needed to synchronize one’s timing

with others for several minutes and with an accuracy of tens of milliseconds.

Second, Tallal and Gaab suggest a general effect of enhanced sequencing skills. They propose that the exposure to music, containing constantly several levels of structures starting from the structure of the piece on a general level and ending in the structure observed within a single bar on a very specific level, would enhance sequencing skills in any task and thus make the child more able to notice sequences appearing in nature, and in tasks. The formula-tion of the sequencing skills is still somewhat open, but it is clear that if such skills are generally enhanced by being exposed to music, the effects would be very beneficial and very general, observable in all possible tasks and offering possibilities for memory functions that are very efficient.

Conclusions

For the whole society, it is beneficial if children have an opportunity to be engaged with music according to their needs and hopes. Neuroscience sheds some light on this question demonstrating several specific and general effects that are observed in children when they play musical instruments, sing, or are engaged in several types of musical activities. This information is also impor-tant to families who are considering offering their child some extracurricular activities. Further, this information is also important for music educators in order to understand that music education is beneficial for all children in sev-eral different ways and at sevsev-eral levels, and that musical education has also benefits outside the acquisition of musical skills. Education programmes in music should be available for children who have high musical aptitude and are willing to work hard on their activity. At the same time, possibilities for engagement with music and musical education should be offered to children who lack some key musical or motoric skills and who may not be willing to spend more than one or two hours per week with their hobby.

For policy makers, especially considering the increasing need to solve is-sues like attention deficits, minor attention regulation problems, learning problems, problems in reading and writing and school bullying, these results should bring a clear message that music education should be available for all children. At schools, the best benefits from music education are obtained with it is not optional, but is offered simultaneously to the whole class, strengthen-ing the social bondstrengthen-ing among the pupils and givstrengthen-ing everybody the possibility to join. Music as a hobby and music at daycare centres and schools should be considered as a key element that supports the learning and attentive skills of individual pupils and social bonding of the group. In my opinion, there is

enough neuroscientific and other evidence to prove that music is beneficial for children.

References

Angelucci, F., Fiore, M., Ricci, E., Padua, L., Sabino, A., & Tonali, P. A. (2007).

Investigating the neurobiology of music: brain-derived neurotrophic factor modulation in the hippocampus of young adult mice. Behavioural Pharma-cology, 18, 491–496.

Anvari, S. H., Trainor, L. J., Woodside, J., & Levy, B. A. (2002). Relations among musical skills, phonological processing, and early reading ability in preschool children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 83, 111–130.

Ashby, F. G., Isen, A. M., & Turken, A. U. (1999). A neuropsychological theory of positive affect and its influence on cognition. Psychological Review, 106, 529–550.

Bengtsson, S. L., Nagy, Z., Skare, S., Forsman, L., Forssberg, H., & Ullén, F. (2005).

Extensive piano practicing has regionally specific effects on white matter de-velopment. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 1148–1150.

Bilhartz, T. D., Bruhn, R. A., & Olson, J. E. (2000). The effect of early music training on child cognitive development. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychol-ogy, 20, 615–636.

Blood A. J., & Zatorre R. J. (2001). Intensely pleasurable responses to music correlate with activity in brain regions implicated in reward and emotion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the U.S.A., 98, 11818–23.

Blood, A. J., Zatorre, R. J., Bermudez, P., & Evans, A. C. (1999). Emotional re-sponses to pleasant and unpleasant music correlate with activity in paralimbic brain regions. Nature Neuroscience, 2, 382–387.

Butzlaff, R. (2000). Can music be used to teach reading? Journal of Aesthetic Educa-tion, 34, 167–78.

Chabris, C. F. (1999). Prelude or requiem for the “Mozart effect”? Nature, 400, 826–

827.

Chan, A. S., Ho, Y. C., & Cheung, M.C. (1998). Music training improves verbal memory. Nature, 396, 128.

Costa-Giomi, E. (2004). Effects of three years of piano instruction on children’s aca-demic achievement, school performance and self-esteem. Psychology of Music, 32, 139–152.

Douglas, S., & Willatts, P. (1994). The relationship between musical ability and liter-acy skills. The Journal of Research in Reading 17, 99–107.

Gardiner, M. F., Fox, A., Knowles, F., & Jeffrey, D. (1996). Learning improved by arts training. Nature, 381, 284.

Gaser, C., & Schlaug, G. (2003). Brain structures differ between musicians and non-musicians. The Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 9240–9245.

Gromko, J. E. (2005). The effect of music instruction on phonemic awareness in beginning readers. Journal of Research in Music Education, 53,199–209

Ho, Y. C., Cheung, M. C., & Chan, A.S. (2003). Music training improves verbal but not visual memory: Cross-sectional and longitudinal explorations in children.

Neuropsychology, 17, 439–450.

Huotilainen, M., Kujala, A., Hotakainen, M., Parkkonen, L., Taulu, S., Simola, J., Nenonen, J., Karjalainen, M., & Näätänen, R. (2005). Short-term memory functions of the human fetus recorded with magnetoencephalography. Neu-roReport, 16, 81–84.

Hutchinson, S., Lee, L. H., Gaab, N., & Schlaug, G. (2003). Cerebellar volume of musicians. Cerebral Cortex, 13, 943–949.

Hyde, K. L., Lerch, J., Norton, A., Forgeard, M., Winner, E., Evans, A.C., & Schlaug, G. (2009). The effects of musical training on structural brain development.

Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1169, 182–186.

Kenealy, P. (1988). Validation of a music mood induction procedure: Some prelimi-nary findings. Cognition & Emotion, 2, 41–48.

Khalfa, S., Dalla Bella, S., Roy, M., Peretz, I., & Lupien, S. J. (2003). Effects of relaxing music on salivary cortisol level after psychological stress. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 999, 374–376.

Kilgour, A. R., Jakobson, L. S., & Cuddy, L. L. (2000). Music training and rate of presentation as mediators of text and song recall. Memory and Cognition, 28, 700–710.

Kuhl, P. (2004). Early language acquisition: cracking the speech code. Nature Re-views Neuroscience, 5, 831–843.

Kujala, T., Karma, K., Ceponiene, R., Belitz, S., Turkkila, P., Tervaniemi, M., &

Näätänen, R. 2001. Plastic neural changes and reading improvement caused by audiovisual training in reading-impaired children. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the U.S.A., 98, 10509–10514.

Lecanuet JP., & Schaal B. (1996). Fetal sensory competencies. European Journal of Obstetrics & Gynecology and Reproductive Biology, 68, 1–23.

Mammarella, N., Fairfield, B., & Cornoldi, C. (2007). Does music enhance cognitive performance in healthy older adults? The Vivaldi effect. Aging Clinical and Experimental Research, 19, 1–6.

Mampe, B., Friederici, A. D., Christophe, A., & Wermke, K. (2009). Newborns’ cry melody is shaped by their native language. Current Biology, 19, 1994–1997.

McMullen, E., & Saffran, J. (2003). Music and language: a developmental perspec-tive. Music Perception 21, 289–311.

Menon, V., & Levitin, D. J. (2005). The rewards of music listening: Response and physiological connectivity of the mesolimbic system. NeuroImage, 28,175–

184.

Milovanov, R., Huotilainen, M., Välimäki, V., Esquef PAA., & Tervaniemi, M.

(2008). Musical aptitude and second language pronunciation skills in school-aged children: Neural and behavioral evidence. Brain Research, 1194, 81–89.

Moreno, S., & Besson, M. (2006). Musical training and language-related brain electri-cal activity in children. Psychophysiology, 43,287–291.

Moreno, S., Marques, C., Santos, A., Santos, M., Castro, S. L., & Besson, M. (2008).

Musical training influences linguistic abilities in 8-year-old children: more evidence for brain plasticity. Cereb Cortex 19, 712–723.

Näätänen, R., Lehtokoski, A., Lennes, M., Cheour, M., Huotilainen, M., Iivonen, A., Vainio, M., Alku, P., Ilmoniemi, R.J., Luuk, A., Allik, J., Sinkkonen, J., &

Alho, K. (1997). Language-specific phoneme representations revealed by electric and magnetic brain responses. Nature, 385, 432–434.

Nelson, D. J., & Barresi, A. L. (1989). Children’s age-related intellectual strategies for dealing with musical and spatial analogical tasks. Journal of Research in Music Education, 37, 93–103.

Nichols, J. A., Jakkamsetti, V. P., Salgado, H., Dinh, L., Kilgard, M.P., & Atzori, M.

(2007). Environmental enrichment selectively increases glutamatergic re-sponses in layer II/III of the auditory cortex of the rat. Neuroscience, 145, 832–840.

Overy, K. (2000). Dyslexia, temporal processing and music: The potential of music as an early learning aid for dyslexic children. Psychology of Music, 28, 218–229.

Overy K., Nicolson R. I., Fawcett A. J., & Clarke E. F. (2003). Dyslexia and music:

measuring musical timing skills. Dyslexia, 9, 18–36.

Papousek, H. (2003). Musicality in infancy research: biological and cultural origins of early musicality. In I. Deliege & J. Sloboda (eds.), Musical beginnings. Ox-ford University Press.

Pascual-Leone, A. (2006). The brain that plays music and is changed by it. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 930, 315–329.

Patel, A. D. (2007). Music, Language and the Brain. New York: Oxford University Press.

Rauscher, F. H., Shaw, G. L., & Ky, C.N. (1993). Music and spatial task-performance.

Nature 365, 611.

Saarikallio, S., & Erkkilä, J. 2007. The role of music in adolescents’ mood regulation.

Psychology of Music, 35(1), 88–109.

Saarikallio, S. (2008). Music in mood regulation: initial scale development. Musicae Scientiae, 12, 291–309.

Schellenberg, E. G. (2006). Exposure to music: The truth about the consequences. In G. E. McPherson (ed.), The child as musician. New York: Oxford University Press, 111–134.

Schellenberg, E. G., Nakata, T., & Tamoto, S. (2007). Exposure to music and cogni-tive performance: tests of children and adults. Psychology of Music, 35, 5–19.

Shahin, A., Roberts, L. E., & Trainor, L. J. (2004). Enhancement of auditory cortical development by musical experience in children. NeuroReport, 15, 1917–1921.

Shestakova, A., Huotilainen, M., Ceponiene, R., & Cheour, M. (2003). Event-related potentials associated with second language learning in children. Clinical Neu-rophysiology, 114, 1507–1512.

Sutoo, D., & Akiyama, K. (2004). Music improves dopaminergic neurotransmission:

demonstration based on the effect of music on blood pressure regulation.

Brain Research, 1016, 255–262.

Tallal, P., & Gaab, N. (2006). Dynamic auditory processing, musical experience and language development. Trends in Neurosciences, 29, 382–390.

Thompson, W. F., Shellenberg, E. G., & Husain, G. (2001). Arousal, mood and the Mozart effect. Psychological Science, 12, 248–251.

Thompson, W. F., Schellenberg, E. G., & Husain, G. (2004). Decoding speech pros-ody: do music lessons help? Emotion, 4, 46–64.

Trainor, L J., & Correga, KA. (2010). Music Acquisition and Effects of Musical Experience. In press.

Trehub, S. E. (2003). Toward a developmental psychology of music. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 999, 402–413.

Wood, J. V., Saltzberg, J. A., & Goldsamt, L.A. (1990). Does affect induce self-focused attention? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 899–

908.

Volkova, A., Trehub, S. E., & Schellenberg, E. G. (2006). Infants’ memory for musi-cal performances. Psychologimusi-cal Science, 9, 583–589.

Questions about the musical development and the