• Ei tuloksia

This means that once a stem is marked with F, this feature should spread to all the suffix vowel s as wel l , both denivational and inflectional

N/A
N/A
Info
Lataa
Protected

Academic year: 2022

Jaa "This means that once a stem is marked with F, this feature should spread to all the suffix vowel s as wel l , both denivational and inflectional"

Copied!
6
0
0

Kokoteksti

(1)

COMMENTS ON SINI KOIVALO-AYDIN'S ARTICLE

0rvokki He.i nåmåiki

Un'i vens'i ty of Helsinki

Abstnact. Sini Koivalo-Aydin questions the autosegmentai method of manking the whole noot for

hanmony, because in Finnish, this would pt'oduce incorrect fonms in derivational suffixes after neutral vowel stems '

She presents an altennative solution, whe¡"e only one root

vowel is manked fot' harmony. If al I de|ivational

suffixes, both product'ive and unpnoductìve, ane taken

into account, 'it is not clear to me how this solution would handle front and back variation in a non-ad-hoc way. Furthermone, if onìy productive suffixes are considened, it nemains to be seen to what extent her solution is needed.

Sini Koivalo-Aydìn's paper" (thìs volume) 'i s thought- provoking, for it challenges an assumption commonly made

by autosegmentaì theorists (e.9. Goldsm'ith, 1985)'

According to this assumption, the autosegmental feature F (=front), when it is pnesent' is associated with the whole lexical entry rather than with individual vowels' A

lexical rule then spreads this featune over" al I vowel posìtions of the stem. This means that once a stem is marked with F, this feature should spread to all the suffix vowel s as wel l , both denivational and inflectional. But disyllabic stems that conta'in only so-

called neutral vowels i,g (e.S. mene-'go')' can pose a problem for the treatment of harmony in derivat'ionaì

suffixes. This is because some suffixes that fol low such stems have back har"mony (e.g. men+o'going'' noun)' whiIe others have front harmony (e.g. men+nyt'gone"'past')'

(2)

Similar pnoblems also arise aften dìsyl labic stems where

the ficst vowel is neutral, the second vowel is å, and the stem-final å is deleted before a derivatìonal suffix (e.g. siirtä+¿i 'to move', siirt+o 'a move', siirt+y+ä 'to

be moved' ).

In her analysis, Kojvalo-Aydin associates the harmonic featune F with the second vowel of disyllabic neutral vowel stems, and not with the whole lexical entry. Moreover, only the marked vowel can spread the

feature F to othen vowels. If the manked vowel is deleted

before the spreading of F takes place, the suffjx vowel

obtains the default value B (=back). But if F is spread

before the deletion of the marked vowel, the suffix vowel gets the feature F. - Koivalo-Aydin first deals with

derivational suffixes attached to neutral vowel stems (3.), and then notes (4. ) that the analysis also appì ies

to "mixed stems" l jke seinä 'wal l' , whìch get a back derivat'ional suffix vowel if the F-marked stem-final ä is deleted, as in sein+us 'anea by the walI'.

There u,as one crucial point in Ko'i valo-Aydin's

analysìs that was uncìeac to me: what detenmines wh ich

rule appl ies first -- the feature spreadìng nule, or the

vowel deletion rule? She mentions the possibiì ity of a

syl lable boundary between the stem and the suffix in the

underlying form me=ne+koon, where e-deletion is the second nule. However, the suffix in me=ne+o, where e is

deleted first, must also be separated by a syllable boundary, since eo is not a possible d'i phtong in Finnish.

This 'lack of clarity makes it difficult to see what predictions the anaìysis makes about suffix vowel s aften

(originally dìsyllabic) stems whene the final vowels have

been deleted. In Kiparsky (1973) -- a starting point fon Koivalo-Aydin -- the frontness vs. backness of the suffix vowel after (surface) monosyl Iabic, neutral vowel stems was determined by the first segment of the suffix: vowel-

(3)

initial suffixes weie predicted to be back, and consonant-in'itial suffixes to be front. However, as

Ringen (1980) has shown, this prediction is not cornect.

For example, the pnoductive suffix -(U)Us is front if the deleted stem-final vowel is å ( e.g., selv¿l 'clean', and

selv+yys 'clarity').

Koivalo-Aydin, as welI as Kiparsky, make no

dist inction between synchronical 1y productìve and

unproductive derivational processes. If a1 I derivational suffixes are grouped together, a great deal of variation aften (sunface monosyl labic) neutral vowel stems must then be accounted for. This is illustnated in (1):

(1)

a ) he'i tt¿i+¿i ' to th row' he i tt+y+ä ' be th nown ' heitt+o 'a throw' hei tt+iö ' nascal ' b) vetä+ä 'to pull' vet+¿iis+t¿i 'pul I quickìy'

vet+o 'a pul ì '

vet+uri 'locomotive'

Al l the suffixes in ( 1) begin with a vowel . Yet, when

attached to the same stem, some of these suffixes contain

a fnont vowel, others have a back vowel. Moreover, some

roots can have both a front and a back version of a suffix, with somewhat different meanings, e.g. mies (miehe-)'man': miehuus'manhood'; miehyys'masculìnìty'.

Stil I others have both vanieties of suffixes without any

effect on the meaning, e.g. pes¿i 'nest': plgE on pesye

'litten'; seinä 'wall': seinus or seinys (dial. ) 'area by the wall'. And finally, the suffix -kkO may appear with a

back vowel even aften dìsyllabic neutral vowel stems,

e.g. villi 'wild': vil'l ikko 'madcap'; kìvi 'stone':

kivikko 'r'ocky ground'; njmi 'name': nimikko 'namesake'.

If the final stem-vowel is marked with F, as in Koivalo-

(4)

Aydin's analysis, and no vowel is deleted, frontness

should spread to the suffix, but it does not.

Another approach to the problem of suffix harmony after neutcal vowel stems is adopted in Anderson ( 1980).

In his extensive study, Anderson looks for general principles that govern suffix harmony. He considers

vowels to have different degrees of harmonic strength.

Thus, the so-cal led neutra'l vowel s ane weakly front harmonic: if the stem contains a back vowel , it always

wins the harmonic battle with a "neutral" vowel. Even individual vowels have different harmonic strengths.

Anderson hypothesizes a harmonic scale, where the hìgh

"neutral " vowel i is at the weak end, and the other

"neutnal " vowel e has a somewhat mone harmonic power.

(For a phonetic basis of a dominance scale of palatal

harmony, see Harms, L982. Hìs scale diffens somewhat fnom

Anderson's).

Two other factocs (besides the harmonic stnength of

root vowels) affect the choice of suffix hanmony in

Anderson's system. One is the quality of intenvening consonants - a factor that t/iik (1975) has also po'inted out. According to Anderson, grave (=back and labial )

consonants (except g) favor back hanmony ìn the suffix.

This could explain, for example, why -kkO tends to have a back vowel in words like nimikko 'namesake'. The other

factor affecting the suffix harmony is the type of the

suffix itself. For instance, out of denivational

suffixes, those that are meaning tnansparent have the

g reatest tendency tourards front harmony. Anderson

succeeds both ìn predicting the hanmony of the suffix vowel quite wel l, and in makìng the variation in the suffix hanmony appear motivated.

Moreover, Anderson points out that panadigms excert pressuce on the harmony of suffixes. Especial ly verbs form "harmonic famil ies", as shown in (2). The fol ìowing

(5)

examples are from Andenson, Appendix A.

I entä+å 'to fly' I enn+ähtä+å

'I evä+tä 'to rest' i ev+¿ihtä+ä

reve+tä 'to rend' rep+¿iìs+t¿i

0ne could also think that a restructuring of the den.ived form may play a nole hene: the suffix -i0, which

denives nouns from venbs, takes front har"mony if the root venb has front harmony, even though the suffixes -0 and

-U are back when they are attached to the same verbal noots. This is illustrated in (3).

(3) keittå+å 'to cook' keitt+iö 'kitchen'

ke'i tt+o ' soup '

elä+ä 'to live' sl+iö 'living

organism'

el+o 'l ife, ì iving' itå+å 'to sprout' it+iö 'a spone'

'i t+u 'a sp rout'

If the suff ix vowel .i is re-ìnterpreted as being part of

the stem, then the front harmonic ö natural ly foi lows.

Let us neturn to Koivalo-Aydin's so'lution. Her aim is to g'i ve a synchronic account of vowel harmony 'i n

suffixes. Thenefone, it would be advisable to consider

only those suffixes that ane synchronical ly productive,

and treat the resu l ts of unproductive derivation as

separate lex'ical items. Karlsson (119S2:250-268) presents

a sunvey of Finnish derivational suffixes and thei n

pr"oductìvity. t{e see that Koivalo-Aydin has unpnoductive

( 2 ) pe'itt¿i+¿i

p i ì rtå+åi

' to cove r' 'do draw'

peitt+y+¿t

p i i rt+y+ä

'to be cove ned' 'to be marked,

outl i ned'

'to fly up,

soa r' 'to have a

rest' 'to tear'

(6)

suffixes in her matenial , e.g. -q, -Us. 0n the other

hand, the reflex'ive-passive suffix -U, and -(U)Us, which

derives property names fnom adjectives and nouns, are

both productive, and they both agree in hanmony with the

the deleted stem-final vowel. This means that the

standard autosegmental method of manking the whoìe root with F wouìd be sufficient, at least fon these suffixes.

It remains to be seen if Koivalo-Aydin's solution is needed in a synchronical ly productive derivation.

Bibl ìography

Anderson, Lloyd 8.1980. Using asymmetnical and gradient

data in the study of vowel harmony. In R. Vago

(ed.), Issues in Vowel Harmony, Benjamins:Amstendam, 271-340.

Goldsmith, John A. 1985. Vowel harmony in Khalka

Mongol ian, Yaka, Finnish and Hungarian. Phonoloqy Yearbook 2t25L-74.

Hanms, Robert T. 1982. What Helmholtz "knew" about

neutnal vowels. Texas Ljnguistic Forum 19:67-90.

Karlsson, Fred t982. Suomen kielen äånne- þ

muotorakenne. WSOY: Helsinki.

Kiparsky, Paul 1973. Phonological ¡"epresenta

Fujimura (ed.), Thnee Dimensions of Theory, TEC:Tokio, 5-136.

Koivalo-Aydin, Sini (this volume) Finnish vowel harmony:

which vowel determines frontness.

R'ingen, Catherine 0. 1980. Finnish vowel harmony: a

closer look. Papen pnesented at the Fourth

Internationa'l Conference of Nordic and Genenal L'i ngu'istics.0slo.

l{iik, Kalevi 1975. Vokaal isoinnun ongelmia. Publ ications

of the Phonetics Department of the Univers ity of

Tu rku . 14.

tions. In 0.

Linquistic

Viittaukset

LIITTYVÄT TIEDOSTOT

It is a storage of all primitive elements, often referred to as “linguistic features.” These can include derivational morphemes, inflectional morphemes, lexical category features,

while the sufñx harmony occasionally takes place, mostly a final front harmony vowel of a disharmonic stem does not enforce the spreading of the feature F to

This means that the Finnish learners, who are used to varying vowel durations in their L1, produced similar vowel durations to those produced by native speakers of English

This connection between the number of a-points and the maximum modulus carries over to transcendental entire functions.. This is a deep property; moreover, some exceptional values α

Updated timetable: Thursday, 7 June 2018 Mini-symposium on Magic squares, prime numbers and postage stamps organized by Ka Lok Chu, Simo Puntanen. &

The stimuli are referred to as target and non-target since the non- native vowel / ʉ / is the target which the participants should learn to perceive and produce in the training

The a priori expectations are in this study that if a subordinate perceives that his superior places higher emphasis on the budget than the subordinate, then there is a need

However, this seems to be a matter of both situational (state) and trait factors (i.e. personality), and if psychopathology plays a role, then its magnitude needs to be assessed