• Ei tuloksia

Diachronic change in Erzya word stress näkymä

N/A
N/A
Info
Lataa
Protected

Academic year: 2022

Jaa "Diachronic change in Erzya word stress näkymä"

Copied!
3
0
0

Kokoteksti

(1)

1 Virittäjä 1/2005, verkkoliite

lektiot

Lectio praecursoria university of helsinki

22ndnovember 2004

The Mordvin languages, Erzya and Moksha, are probably the closest linguistic relatives to the Finnic group. An unusual feature of Erzya is its word stress, which differs from at least most other languages insofar as speakers may lay stress on any syllable they wish. It is difficult to fit such word stress into any existing typology. I have called this type of word stress preferential.

The aim of this research has been, in the first instance, to resolve five matters.

(1) The extent to which Erzya word stress has changed over the last two hundred years; a comparison was made with extant documentary evidence, (2) whether or not

Diachronic change in Erzya

word stress

DENNIS ESTILL

Erzya word stress is systematic, (3) wheth- er there has been similar change in Moksha word stress, (4) the acoustic parameters of Erzya word stress and, (5) whether Proto- Finno-Ugrian word stress has been reflected in the prosodic development of the Mordvin languages.

The first matter has been resolved on the basis of historical documentation, the so- called Damaskin Dictionary from the year 1785 (published in Feoktistov 1971) and a Short Catechism dated 1788 (full text in Feoktistov 1968). An analysis of these showed that slightly over two hundred years ago word stress was, for the most part, sta- tistically on the second syllable, whereas today it is on the first syllable with almost the same regularly. This research also con- firmed that, whereas today word stress is

(2)

2 near the beginning of the word, two hundred years ago it was, on average, nearer the end of the word. The figure below shows the development of word stress since 1785–

1788. The bends in the curves at a point in between are based on evidence left by Heik- ki Paasonen (1903). This figure demon- strates how rapidly word stress can change.

As far as the next matter is concerned, it must be said that although Erzya word stress has earlier generally been on the se- cond syllable, and nowadays it is predomi- nantly on the first syllable, the analysed ma- terial clearly demonstrates how then and now the speaker is free to place stress in ac- cordance with personal preferences. The position of word stress is not structurally de- termined, and the speaker may, in general, stress any syllable desired. In this respect, Erzya is something of an exceptional lan- guage.

The third point concerns the other Mordvin language, Moksha. It appears from the research that a similar develop- ment to Erzya has taken place Moksha, at least since the year 1785. It would seem that Moksha has gone ahead of Erzya, as it were, and that word stress is coming to rest regu- larly on the first syllable. However, in the case of Moksha it is difficult to draw any definite conclusions on account of the frag- mentary nature of the evidence.

The acoustic correlates of modern Erzya word stress were also studied. Although

fundamental frequency (F0) is normally regarded as the most important acoustic parameter in the production of word stress, it would seem that in the case of Erzya in- tensity measured as amplitude is a more significant feature than F0, as is also acous- tic length (duration). On the other hand, F0 is evidently a more important parameter in producing sentence stress. In this respect also Erzya is probably a less common lan- guage.

Finally, what can be said about word stress in the Proto-Finno-Ugrian language in the light of the recent historical develop- ment of Mordvin? The prosodic develop- ment of these languages is far from easy to determine. Nevertheless, no findings in the research material imply that first syllable word stress has been inherited from Proto- Mordvin or some other proto-language.

Similar statements could also be made re- garding the other Finno-Ugrian languages in this region of Russia.

Furthermore, this research has shown, or at least drawn attention to the fact, that nowadays disyllabic words are somewhat 0

20 40 60 80 100

1785 2000

%

1st syl.

2nd syl.

3rd syl.

4th syl.

%

(3)

3 regularly initially stressed, although word stress is rather evenly distributed in the case of polysyllabic words. On the other hand, there were conflicting findings in analyses of the relative attraction of different vowels.

The results also supported an assertion that today lists are read with initial stress.

The philological investigation of the extant documents was based on a statisti- cal analysis of databases created from these for that specific purpose, and the present- day material has been examined by meas- uring the acoustic parameters.

Although I have attempted to describe the phonological features of the Erzya lan- guage and its dialects, less attention has been paid to morphology and syntax. The reason for this is that at least descriptions of these latter are already available to some extent in English, and phonology has been closer to the subject at hand, although this is not to deny the relevance of morphology and syntax. Therefore, this does not mean that studying stems and suffixes would not increase the understanding of Erzya word stress. This is something to take into con- sideration in the future.

DENNIS ESTILL Diachronic change in Erzya word stress. Mémoires de la société Finno- Ougrienne 246. Helsinki: Société Finno-Ougrienne.

Sähköposti: dennis.estill@helsinki.fi

In summary, it can be stated that even though the feature of word stress exists and has existed in Erzya historically, in the fi- nal instance it is and has been realised through individual speaker preferences.

This can be termed preferential word stress, because the speaker’s word stress pattern is determined statistically by personal prefer- ences. Bearing this in mind, we can con- clude that during the last two hundred years Erzya has been a language whose word stress could only be partially predicted, and which during this period at least has never had a comprehensive structural word stress system.

References

FEOKTISTOV, A. P.: 1968: Istoki mordovskoy pis'mennosti. Moskva: Nauka.

–––– 1971: Russko-mordovskiy slovar': Iz istorii otechestvennoy leksikografii.

Moskva: Nauka.

PAASONEN, HEIKKI 1903: Morwinische Laut- lehre. Mémoires de la Société Finno- Ougrienne 22. Helsinki: Société Fin- no-Ougrienne.

Viittaukset

LIITTYVÄT TIEDOSTOT

It means that we have something in common The word we does not have any meaning anymore The using of the word we I am always “us/we”.. for outside partners the word “we” means

In this study, reflexive passive verbs from three Finno-Ugric languages (Erzya, Moksha, and Mari) and one Turkic language (Chuvash) have been analysed and then used to make some

For evaluation of stress response, 54 VLBW and 40 control participants underwent a standardized psychosocial stress test, the Trier Social Stress Test (TSST). In conjunction

In this context, the word tetrameter must be viewed as analogous to Bede’s use of the word in conjunction with the iambic dimeter: for Bede, the metre has two alternating lines of

Yksi rivi Napsauta valinta-alueella (asiakirjan vasemmassa reunassa) rivin kohdalla. Hiiren osoitin on valkoinen yläoikealle osoittava nuoli. Useita rivejä Vedä

Each word in the input is represented by a word node connected by excitatory links to sense nodes representing the different possible senses for that word in the

In the local cases of the definiqe declension Erzya, and Moksha in particular, uses both synthetic case endings and the separate postposition ez and the case ending,

The lexical access tasks used in the current study (single word recognition and word retrieval) lack the contextual clues for priming to happen, but difficulties in suppressing